A shared numerical representation for action and perception

  • Anobile G
  • Arrighi R
  • Togoli I
  • et al.
N/ACitations
Citations of this article
66Readers
Mendeley users who have this article in their library.

Abstract

Humans and other species have perceptual mechanisms dedicated to estimating approximate quantity: a sense of number. Here we show a clear interaction between self-produced actions and the perceived numerosity of subsequent visual stimuli. A short period of rapid finger-tapping (without sensory feedback) caused subjects to underestimate the number of visual stimuli presented near the tapping region; and a period of slow tapping caused overestimation. The distortions occurred both for stimuli presented sequentially (series of flashes) and simultaneously (clouds of dots); both for magnitude estimation and forced-choice comparison. The adaptation was spatially selective, primarily in external, real-world coordinates. Our results sit well with studies reporting links between perception and action, showing that vision and action share mechanisms that encode numbers: a generalized number sense, which estimates the number of self-generated as well as external events.Humans and many other animals have the ability to make spontaneous and rapid estimates of the approximate number of items that they can see. This sense of number, or “numbersense”, is particularly important in humans, as evidence suggests that it lays the groundwork for acquiring mathematical skills.Researchers have many questions about numbersense. Is it a kind of perception? Or does it require more active thought, like counting? Do people have the same sense of number when they view, hear or touch items that depict the same number? Having a sense of number is essential for carrying out certain actions, like the following the steps in a dance, but the connection between action and numbersense is not entirely clear.A process called adaptation means that viewing specific stimuli for a period of time can affect what people think they see subsequently. For example, viewing large numbers of dots makes subsequent smaller groups of dots seem like they contain fewer dots than they actually do. Anobile, Arrighi et al. have now investigated the link between action and numbersense by asking volunteers to tap one hand either rapidly or slowly in one spot for a short time. The volunteers were then shown a series flashes or a cloud of dots in the region where they had been tapping and asked to estimate the number of flashes or dots.After fast tapping, the volunteers greatly underestimated the numbers of flashes or dots that they saw; after slow tapping, they overestimated the numbers. However, if the images were shown far away from where the volunteers had been tapping, their estimates were more accurate.Overall, the results suggest that adaptation is controlled by space-specific sensory mechanisms rather than some kind of active counting. Furthermore, numbersense appears to have a generalized form that is shared by the brain regions responsible for perception and action. Because numbersense and mathematical ability are linked, this strong connection between action and number perception may have important implications for understanding and treating math-related learning disabilities. Anobile, Arrighi et al. next plan to study how movement-driven adaptation affects numbersense in children and adults with these conditions.

Cite

CITATION STYLE

APA

Anobile, G., Arrighi, R., Togoli, I., & Burr, D. C. (2016). A shared numerical representation for action and perception. ELife, 5. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.16161

Register to see more suggestions

Mendeley helps you to discover research relevant for your work.

Already have an account?

Save time finding and organizing research with Mendeley

Sign up for free