Increasing Effectiveness of Charitable Mail Solicitations Using Negative Emotions

  • Patil V
  • Administration B
N/ACitations
Citations of this article
5Readers
Mendeley users who have this article in their library.

Abstract

Purpose In 2006, U.S. households received over 700 million mail requests for donations and donated over $295 billion (Giving USA 2007; Household Dairy Study 2006). As a result of the ever increasing needs for funds for charities, not only in the US but across globe, there is a need to increase the effectiveness of mail appeals from charities. Since the use of emotions in appeals for mail solicitations is common, we explore how two frequently used negative emotions, guilt and empathy (Batson 1991; Coulter and Pinto 1995) can increase the effectiveness of mail appeals. Specifically, we attempt to answer two key questions. First, what motivates people to donate when they are exposed to guilt% or empathy% inducing mail appeals? Second, are the two emotions appropriate for all solicitations, independent of the involvement of the potential donor with the charity? Theoretical Background Appraisal theories (e.g., Lazarus 1991) suggest that emotions are motivational responses to appraisals of environmental stimuli, which lead to behaviors deemed suitable to cope with these stimuli (Brehm 1999). Guilt has been defined as a negative, moral, and self-referential emotion, which involves an internal attribution about a negative outcome of a particular task (Tangney and Dearing 2002). Empathy involves the ability to take the perspective of others, such that generated feelings are focused on others and not on self (Batson 1991). In the context of nonprofit mail solicitations inducing empathy, since the state of a beneficiary is negative (for example, a disabled person in need of assistance), we classify empathy as a negative emotion (Lazarus 1991). Based on past literature (e.g., Aaker and Williams 1998), we also assume empathy, sympathy, pity, and compassion to be similar emotions. Motives for helping have been classified as being egoistic and altruistic (e.g., Batson 2002). These differ with respect to the ultimate objective of the act of helping. Egoistic motives are geared towards enhancing the welfare of self by either seeking rewards, or avoiding punishment, or reducing distress. An altruistic motive, in contrast, has the welfare of the needy as the ultimate goal. Since motives for helping are unobservable, researchers have inferred those using different variables such as the viability of escape from helping (Batson 1991). This suggests that an individual with an egoistic motive will help only when escape from helping is difficult because s/he would weigh the costs and/or benefits of helping. In contrast, an altruistically motivated individual will help even when escape from helping is easy. Guilt induction leads to a self or socially imposed censure and in order to cope with it, people are likely to behave in one of three different ways: (a) try to help; (b) know if others have helped and if help has indeed been provided, relieve their guilt because their help is no longer needed; (c) psychologically escape from the need situation (Batson 1991; Lazarus 1991; Tangney and Dearing 2002). As guilt focuses one's attention on one's own state, any helping that occurs is likely to be because of an egoistic motive. Research on guilt also suggests that the capacity to experience guilt declines as the emotional distance between individuals increases (Baumeister et al. 1995). As a result, guilt is more likely to be successful in eliciting helping behavior for a high involvement charity. When low involvement charities induce guilt, people would infer such appeals to be manipulative and respond negatively to them (Friestad and Wright 1994). This would lead to reduced helping, which would be further reduced when escape from helping is made difficult. Since empathy-inducing appeals focus one's attention on the plight of the needy, any help that is generated is likely to be because of an altruistic motive as long as the need to help is perceived.… [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]

Cite

CITATION STYLE

APA

Patil, V. H., & Administration, B. (2008). Increasing Effectiveness of Charitable Mail Solicitations Using Negative Emotions. Advances in Consumer Research, VIII(Volume VIII), 350–351.

Register to see more suggestions

Mendeley helps you to discover research relevant for your work.

Already have an account?

Save time finding and organizing research with Mendeley

Sign up for free